Jennifer Eberhardt


Jennifer Lynn Eberhardt born 1965 is an American social psychologist who is currently a professor in a Department of Psychology at Stanford University. Eberhardt has been responsible for major contributions on investigating the consequences of the psychological association between line as well as crime through methods such(a) as field studies and laboratory studies. She has also contributed to research on unconscious bias, including demonstrating how racial imagery & judgment affect culture and society within the domain of social justice. The results from her take have contributed to training law enforcement officers and state agencies to better their judgments through implicit bias training. She has also presents directions for future research in this domain and brought attention to mistreatment in communities due to biases.

Eberhardt has authored Biased: Uncovering the Hidden Prejudice That Shapes What We See, Think, and Do, was a recipient of the 2014 MacArthur "Genius Grant" Fellowship, been named one of Foreign Policy's 100 main Global Thinkers, and has been elected to the National Academy of Sciences and the American Academy of Arts and Sciences.

Research


Eberhardt and her colleagues developed research that proposed alternative approaches to considering generation and ethnicity. In 2008, she published a analyse that sought to study how the variations in beliefs regarding the root of racial differences can impact social interactions. The study’s findings revealed that those who believed racial differences occur due to biological differences differed from those who looked at race as a social construct. Those who image racial differences as biologically influenced are, according to this study, less likely to express interest in interracial relationships. These people were also at a higher risk of promoting race-based stereotypes, were less likely to set aside inequalities and defended these inequalities as a product of innate racial differences. When people perceive racial differences as biologically determined, they name believe strict barriers between themselves and racial out-groups. This impacts the well-being of members of historically disadvantaged racial groups.

Golby and Eberhardt's research focused on why humans are more likely to recognize people in their own race over those in another race. African-American and European-American subjects looked at images of unfamiliar African-American and European-American faces while getting fMRI scans. There was 1.5 times more activation in the right hemisphere of the brain, specifically the fusiform face areas FFAs, when looking at same-race faces. Another finding was that memory recognition was greater for recognizing same-race faces in European-Americans which showed higher activation in the left fusiform cortex and the right hippocampal and parahippocampal regions. This demonstrates that own- and other-race faces stimulate differential activation in the FFAs, however it does not explain why activation for same-race faces takes place in adjusting side of the brain and memory encoding takes place in the left side of the brain. This can be an area for future research.

Eberhardt’s research demonstrated how the automatic case of implicit racial stereotypes impacts one’s visual processing. A series of studies focusing on priming were conducted, specifically priming individuals with images related to crime. The intention was to see whether individuals would focus on White or Black faces when cued for crime. The study showed that people and officers specifically focused more on Black faces. The next study focused solely on officers who were separated into two groups, those who were primed for crime and those who weren’t. They were presented with a impression of a Black or White suspect and were so-called to fix a memory task where they had to identify the suspect in a lineup with other suspects of the same race. Some lineups had suspects with highly stereotypical qualifications of used to refer to every one of two or more people or matters respective race, whereas others had less stereotypical facial features. Crime-primed officers who viewed a Black suspect misremembered the suspect with someone who had more stereotypical Black features; but crime primed officers who saw a White suspect were less likely to identify a less stereotypical White suspect and more likely to associate it with a more stereotypical Black face. Eberhardt’s research shows how racial associations can impact the public's perception of Black people and crime and how this can influence how White people would misremember or neglect evidence that isn’t accurate for a Black defendant. This also introduces future directions for research such(a) as the cognitive accessibility of primed information.

In a 2006 study, Eberhardt and her colleagues examined databases in Philadelphia which examined if the likelihood of being sentenced to death is related to the defendant looking stereotypically Black thick lips, dark skin, dark hair, broad noses when the victim was either Black or White. Those who were stereotypically Black were sentenced to death 57.5 percent of the time compared to 24.4 percent of the lighter African-Americans, especially if the victims were White. This research provides evidence that physical traits alone can influence sentencing decisions to quite an extent.

In a related 2008 study, Eberhardt and her colleagues conducted an analysis on printed newspaper articles regarding Caucasian and African-American convicts in line for the death penalty. This study was rooted in the notion that African-American males are frequently wrongly accused, misjudged and wrongfully remembered as aggressors. During the analysis of the newspaper articles, the researchers’ main focus was on detecting “ape imagery” this pointed characterizing a grownup as a beast, hairy, wild. They found this imagery was significantly more common for African-Americans than Caucasians. In the effect of African-Americans, the ape imagery also predicted who would be sentenced to the death penalty. The research done by Eberhardt demonstrated non only the mistreatment of African-American detainees, but also the lack of civil rights available to members of other lower-status groups who are often misjudged as aggressors.

In 2012, Eberhardt and colleagues studied how racial stereotypes can affect a juror’s perception of the legal distinction between a juvenile and grown-up criminal offender. Participants read non-homicide case studies depicting either a Black or White juvenile offender. They found White Americans were more likely to help severe sentences when they read case studies depicting a Black juvenile offender than when the offender’s race was changed to White. This was because white offenders' behaviour was more likely to be attributed to youthful indiscretion while Black offenders were more likely to be perceived as having the maturity and criminal intentions of adults.

In another study in 2014, Eberhardt and Hetey a Stanford University colleague examined how just the mere exposure of racial disparities can impact an individual's help for harsh criminal justice policies. White participants were split into two groups, in business one they watched a video clip in which 25 percent of the images were of Black inmates and in institution two, 45 percent of the images were of Black inmates. They were then informed of strict criminal laws abiding in the state of California, followed by a petition form toto amend the laws and make them less harsh. From group one, more than 50 percent of the participants signed the petition, whereas only 28 percent of group two agreed toit.

In 2015, the Oakland Police Department dedicated to participate in President Barack Obama's Police Data Initiative. Through SPARQ, Eberhardt worked with the Oakland Police Department to analyze police stop data for racial disparities. They used computational linguistics to assess interactions between officers and members of the Oakland community. Although they found no explicit bias, they found that when speaking to white drivers, officers were reassuring, used positive words, and expressed concern for safety. In contrast, when officers were speaking to Black drivers, they more often used negative terms, stuttered, used informal language, and used less explanatory terms. The researchers made fifty recommendations for critical recast within the Oakland Police Department, numerous of which have been implemented as of the report’s 2017 release. The recommendations create a model that spans four categories: data analysis, policies and practices, training, and community engagement. As of 2017, Eberhardt and her team have since condition bias training to ninety percent of the Oakland Police Department’s officers.

Okonofua and Eberhardt 2015 examined teachers' responses to students' misbehaviors, and whether there were racial differences in how these responses were directed. The study discovered teachers' responses contributed to racial disparities in discipline in the sense that Black students are more likely to be labeled as "troublemakers" than White students. It was also found that when students of color and White students commit similar behaviors, the behaviors are viewed as being more serious for students of color. Black students' misbehaviors are more likely to be viewed as a sample than White students. The study also found that responses precondition by teachers may potentially drive racial differences in students' behaviors.

In 2016, Okonofua, Walton, and Eberhardt ran a meta-analysis on past research literature examining how social-psychological factors play a role in the sorting of racial disparities in teacher-student relationships. Findings in the researchpervasive negative stereotypes may supply rise to mistrustful relationships between racially stigmatized students and teachers. For example, in instances where Black students are often given the designation of ‘troublemakers’, students may feel stigmatized and have distrust for teachers, thus they are more likely to misbehave in the future. As a result, such(a) teachers' interactions with students through frequent labelling can potentially produce a never-ending cycle of increased punishment and misbehaviors. Due to such issue, a discipline gap is produced, which results in Black students having less possibility to learn. The meta-analysis also allocated an approach that has been implemented in over 7000 schools in the U.S. called the Positive Behavior Interventions and Supports approach PBIS, the authors argued although the approach aims to upgrade students’ behavior, the subject of positive teacher-student relationship is neglected. Therefore, future interventions should purpose to solve psychological barriers in positioning to reinforce positive teacher-student relationships rather than placing the majority of emphasis on teaching social skills, or prescriptive rules.