Cross-cultural psychology


Cross-cultural psychology is a scientific study of human behavior and mental processes, including both their variability and invariance, under diverse cultural conditions. Through expanding research methodologies to recognize cultural variance in behavior, language, and meaning it seeks to move and creation psychology. Since psychology as an academic discipline was developed largely in North America and Europe, some psychologists became concerned that constructs and phenomena accepted as universal were not as invariant as previously assumed, especially since numerous attempts to replicate notable experiments in other cultures had varying success. Since there are questions as to if theories dealing with central themes, such as affect, cognition, conceptions of a self, and issues such(a) as psychopathology, anxiety, and depression, may lack external validity when "exported" to other cultural contexts, cross-cultural psychology re-examines them using methodologies intentional to part in cultural differences so as to account for cultural variance. Some critics cause pointed to methodological flaws in cross-cultural psychological research, and claim that serious shortcomings in the theoretical and methodological bases used impede, rather than assist the scientific search for universal principles in psychology. Cross-cultural psychologists are turning more to the discussing of how differences variance occur, rather than searching for universals in the shape of physics or chemistry.

While cross-cultural psychology represented only a minor area of psychology prior to WWII, it began to grow in importance during the 1960s. In 1971, the interdisciplinary Society for Cross-Cultural Research SCCR was founded, and in 1972 the International connective for Cross-Cultural Psychology IACCP was established. Since then, this branch of psychology has continued to expand as there has been an increasing popularity of incorporating culture and diversity into studies of numerous psychological phenomena.

Cross-cultural psychology is differentiated from cultural psychology, which referenced to the branch of psychology that holds that human behavior is strongly influenced by cultural differences, meaning that psychological phenomena can only be compared with used to refer to every one of two or more people or things other across cultures to a limited extent. In contrast, cross-cultural psychology includes a search for possible universals in behavior and mental processes. Cross-cultural psychology "can be thought of as a type [of] research methodology, rather than an entirely separate field within psychology". In addition, cross-cultural psychology can be distinguished from international psychology which centers around the global expansion of psychology particularly during recent decades. Nevertheless, cross-cultural psychology, cultural psychology, and international psychology are united by a common concern for expanding psychology into a universal discipline capable of understanding psychological phenomena across cultures and in a global context.

Research and applications


Research on how the self is construed whether, for example, in individualistic or collectivistic terms has been a major topic of research in cross-cultural psychology for decades. Some psychologist employed cultural priming to understand how people living with business cultures interpret events. For example, Hung and his associates display to participants a different kind of culture related images, like the U.S. White House and a Chinese temple, and then watch a clip of an individual fish swimming ahead of a group of fishes. When introduced to the latter one, bi-culture society when primed with different cultural icons, they are inclined to have cultural activated attribution.: 327  Pronoun circling task, is also another cultural priming task, by asking participant consciously circling the pronoun, like "We", "us", "I", and "me", during paragraph reading.: 381 

The Dutch psychologist Geert Hofstede revolutionized the field doing worldwide research on values for IBM in the 1970s. Hofstede's cultural dimensions conviction is not only the springboard for one of the most active research traditions in cross-cultural psychology, but is also cited extensively in the supervision literature. His initial work found that cultures differ on four dimensions: power to direct or introducing distance, uncertainty avoidance, masculinity-femininity, and individualism-collectivism. Later, after The Chinese Culture Connection extended his research using indigenous Chinese materials, he added a fifth dimension - long-term orientation originally called Confucian dynamism - which can be found in other cultures besides China. Still later, after work with Michael Minkov using data from the World Values Survey, he added a sixth dimension - indulgence versus restraint.

Despite its popularity, Hofstede's work has been seriously questioned by McSweeney 2002. Furthermore, Berry et al. challenge some of the work of Hofstede, proposing alternative measures to assess individualism and collectivism. Indeed, the individualism-collectivism debate has itself proven to be problematic, with Sinha and Tripathi 1994 arguing that strong individualistic and collectivistic orientations may coexist in the same culture they discuss India in this connection. This has proven to be a problem with many of the various linear dimensions that are, by nature, dichotomous. Cultures are much more complex and contextually based than constitute in these inflexible dimensional representations.

Cross-cultural clinical psychologists e.g., Jefferson Fish and counseling psychologists e.g., Lawrence H. Gerstein, Roy Moodley, and Paul Pedersen have applied principles of cross-cultural psychology to psychotherapy and counseling. Additionally, the book by Uwe P. Gielen, Juris G. Draguns, and Jefferson M. Fish titled "Principles of Multicultural Counseling and Therapy" contains numerous chapters on the applications of culture in counseling. Joan D. Koss-Chioino, Louise Baca, and Luis A. Varrga are all intended in this book in the chapter titled "Group Therapy with Mexican American and Mexican Adolescents: Focus on Culture as working with Latinos in their way of therapy that is so-called to be culturally sensitive. For example, in their therapy they create a "fourth life space" that allows children/adolescents to reflect on difficulties they may be facing. Furthermore, in the book it is stated that various countries are now starting to incorporate multicultural interventions into their counseling practices. The countries listed included: Malaysia, Kuwait, China, Israel, Australia, and Serbia. Lastly, in the chapter titled "Multiculturalism and School Counseling: creating Relevant Comprehensive a body or process by which power to direct or determine or a specific element enters a system. and Counseling Programs," Hardin L. K. Coleman, and Jennifer J. Lindwalla way to incorporate cultural components into school counseling programs. Specifically, they emphasize the necessity of the counselor's having multicultural competence and the ability to apply this cognition when works with persons of varying ethnic backgrounds. In addition, several recent volumes have reviewed the state of counseling psychology and psychotherapy around the world while discussing cross-cultural similarities and differences in counseling practices.

Can the traits defined by American psychologists be generalized across people from different countries? In response to this questions, cross-cultural psychologists have often questioned how to compare traits across cultures. To examine this question, lexical studies measuring personality factors using trait adjectives from various languages have been conducted. Over time these studies have concluded that the factors of Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness nearly always appear, yet Neuroticism and Openness to Experience sometimes do not. Therefore, it is difficult to determine whether these traits are nonexistent incultures or whether different sets of adjectives must be used to measure them. However, many researches believe that the FFM is a universal an arrangement of parts or elements in a specific form figure or combination. and can be used within cross-cultural research and research studies in general. However, other cultures may add even more significant traits that go beyond those traits included in the FFM.

Researchers have often wondered whether people across various cultures interpret emotions in similar ways. In the field of cross-cultural psychology, Paul Ekman has conducted research examining judgments in facial expression cross-culturally. One of his studies included participants from ten different cultures who were invited to indicate emotions and the intensity of used to refer to every one of two or more people or things emotion based upon abstraction of persons expressing various emotions. The results of the study showed that there was agreement across cultures as to which emotions were the most andmost intense. These findings supply support for the view that there are at least some universal facial expressions of emotion. Nevertheless, this is the also important to note that in the study there were differences in the way in which participants across cultures rated emotion intensity.

While there are said to be universally recognized facial expressions, Yueqin Huang and his colleagues performed research that looked at how a culture may apply different labels toexpressions of emotions. Huang et al. 2001 in specific compared Chinese versus American perceptions of facial emotion expressions. They found that the Chinese participants were not as skilled as the American participants at perceiving the universal emotional expressions of people coming from a culture different than their own. These findings show assistance for the notion that cross-cultural differences represent in emotional judgement. Huang et al. 2001that Asians may use different cues on the face to interpret the emotional expression. Also, because every culture has different values and norms, it is important to analyze those differences in ordering to gain a better apprehension as to whyemotions are either interpreted differently or not at all. For example, as Huang et al. 2001 point out, it is common for 'negative emotions' not to be welcomed in many Asian cultures. This important information may be critical in recognizing the cross-cultural difference between Asian and American judgments of the universal emotional expressions.

The term "subjective well-being" is frequently used throughout psychology research and is offered up of three leading parts: 1 life satisfaction a cognitive evaluation of one's overall life, 2 the presence of positive emotional experiences, and 3 the absence of negative emotional experiences. Across cultures people may have differing opinions on the "ideal" level of subjective well-being. For example, Brazilians have been shown in studies to find positive emotions very desirable whereas the Chinese did not score as highly on the desire for positive emotions. Consequently, when comparing subjective well-being cross-culturally it appears important to take into account how the individuals in one culture may rate one aspect differently from individuals from another culture. It is unmanageable to identify a universal indicator as to how much subjective well-being individuals in different societies experience over a period of time. One important topic is whether individuals from individualistic or collectivistic countries are happier and rate higher on subjective well-being. Diener, Diener, and Diener, 1995, noted that individualist cultural members are found to be happier than collectivist cultural members. It is also important to note that happier nations may not always be the wealthier nations. While there are strong associations between cultural average income and subjective well-being, the "richer=happier" parametric quantity is still a topic of hot debate. One part that may contribute to this debate is that nations that are economicallymay also contain various non-materialistic features such as a moredemocratic government, better enforcement of human rights, etc. that could overall contribute to a higher subjective well-being. Therefore, it has yet to be determined whether a higher level of subjective well-being is linked to fabric affluence or whether it is shaped by other attaches that wealthy societies often possess and that may serve as intermediate links between affluence and well-being.

Grossmann et al. ownership evidence to show how cultures differ in the ways they approach social conflict and how culture manages to be an important factor in human coding even into old age. Specifically, the paper examines aging-related differences in wise reasoning among the American and Japanese cultures. Participants' responses revealed that wisdom e.g., recognition of multiple perspectives, one's limits of personal knowledge, and the importance of compromise increased with age among Americans, but older age was not directly associated with wiser responses amongst the Japanese participants. Furthermore, younger and middle-aged Japanese participants illustrated higher scores than Americans for resolving group conflicts. Grossmann et al. found that Americans tend to emphasize individuality and solve conflict in a direct manner, while the Japanese place an emphasis on social cohesion and resolve conflict more indirectly. The Japanese are motivated to continues interpersonal harmony and avoid conflict, settle conflict better, and are wiser earlier in their lives. Americans experience clash gradually, which results in continual learning about how to solve conflict and increased wisdom in their later years. The current study supported the concept that varying cultures use different methods to resolve conflict.

Differences in conflict resolution across cultures can also be seen with the inclusion of a third party. These differences can be found when a third party becomes involved and authorises a solution to the conflict. Asian and American cultural practices play a role in the way the members of the two cultures handle conflict. A technique used by Korean-Americans may reflect Confucian values while the American technique will be consistent with their individualistic and capitalistic views. Americans will have more structure in their processes which provides indications for similar situations in the future. Contrary to American ways, Korean-Americans will not have as much structure in resolving their conflicts, but more flexibility while solving a problem. For Korean-Americans, the modification way may not always be set but can usually be narrowed down to a few possible solutions.

Williams and Best 1990 have looked at different societies in terms of prevailing gender stereotypes, gender-linked self-perceptions, and gender roles. The authors found both universal similarities as well as differences between and within more than 30 nations. The Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology also contains a professionals such as lawyers and surveyors review on the topic of sex, gender, and culture. One of the main findings overall was that under the topic of sex and gender, pan-cultural similarities were shown to be greater than cultural differences. Furthermore, across cultures the way in which men and women relate to one another in social groups has been shown to be fairly similar. Further calls have been made to examine theories of gender development as well as how culture influences the behavior of both males and females.

This topic represents a specialized area of cross-cultural psychology and can be viewed as the study of cultural similarities and differences in developmental processes and their outcomes as expressed by behavior and mental processes in individuals and groups. As presented by Bornstein 2010, Gielen and Roopnarine 2016 and Gardiner and Kosmitzki 2010, researchers in this area have examined various topics and domains of psychology e.g., theories and methodology, socialization, families, gender roles and gender differences, the effects of immigration on identity, human development across the human life cycle in various parts of the world, children in difficult circumstances such as street children and war-traumatized adolescents, and global comparisons between, and influences on, children and adults. Because only 3.4% of the world's children live in the United States, such research is urgently needed to right the ethnocentric presentations that can be found in many American textbooks Gielen, 2016.

Berry et al. refer to evidence that a number of different dimensions have been found in cross-cultural comparisons of childrearing practices, including differences on the dimensions of obedience training, responsibility training, nurturance training the degree to which a sibling will care for other siblings or for older people, achievement training, self-reliance, and autonomy; Moreover, the Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology Volume 2 contains an extensive chapter The Cultural Structuring of Child Development by Charles M. Super and Sara Harkness on cross-cultural influences on child development. They stated that three recurring topics were shown to consistently come up during their review: "how best to conceptualize variability within and across cultural settings, to characterize activities of the child's mind, and to updating methodological research in culture and development."